File size: 20,518 Bytes
6fa4bc9
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
61
62
63
64
65
66
67
68
69
70
71
72
73
74
75
76
77
78
79
80
81
82
83
84
85
86
87
88
89
90
91
92
93
94
95
96
97
98
99
100
101
102
103
104
105
106
107
108
109
110
111
112
113
114
115
116
117
118
119
120
121
122
123
124
125
126
127
128
129
130
131
132
133
134
135
136
137
138
139
140
141
142
143
144
145
146
147
148
149
150
151
152
153
154
155
156
157
158
159
160
161
162
163
164
165
166
167
168
169
170
171
172
173
174
175
176
177
178
179
180
181
182
183
184
185
186
187
188
189
190
191
192
193
194
195
196
197
198
199
200
201
202
203
204
205
206
207
208
209
210
211
212
213
214
215
216
217
218
219
220
221
222
223
224
225
226
227
228
229
230
231
232
233
234
235
236
237
238
239
240
241
242
243
244
245
246
247
248
249
250
251
252
253
254
255
256
257
258
259
260
261
262
263
264
265
266
267
268
{
    "paper_id": "M91-1010",
    "header": {
        "generated_with": "S2ORC 1.0.0",
        "date_generated": "2023-01-19T03:15:28.027368Z"
    },
    "title": "ITP INTERPRETEXT SYSTEM: MUC-3 TEST RESULTS AND ANALYSI S",
    "authors": [
        {
            "first": "Kathleen",
            "middle": [],
            "last": "Dahlgre",
            "suffix": "",
            "affiliation": {
                "laboratory": "",
                "institution": "Carol Lord Hajime Wada Joyce McDowel l Edward P . Stabler",
                "location": {
                    "region": "Jr"
                }
            },
            "email": ""
        }
    ],
    "year": "",
    "venue": null,
    "identifiers": {},
    "abstract": "",
    "pdf_parse": {
        "paper_id": "M91-1010",
        "_pdf_hash": "",
        "abstract": [],
        "body_text": [
            {
                "text": "Intelligent Text Processing is a small start-up company participating in the MUC-3 exercise fo r the first time this year . Our system, Interpretext, is based on a prototype text understandin g system. With three full-time and three part-time people, dividing time between MUC-3 and othe r contract projects, ITP made maximum use of modest resources . Figure 1 . The ITP system was second highest in precision (46%) when all templates were considered, and at the same time achieved a credible recall percentage (20%) . Our overgeneration rate was second best (34%) . ITP was a very close second in both precisio n and overgeneration, as the top percentages were 48 and 33 to ITP's 46 and 34 . The major limitin g factor in ITP's MUC-3 performance was parser failure. We are building a parser with wide coverage and a comprehensive approach to disambiguation . Because our parser is not yet complete, in order to participate in the MUC-3 exercise we used a parser on loan .",
                "cite_spans": [],
                "ref_spans": [
                    {
                        "start": 352,
                        "end": 360,
                        "text": "Figure 1",
                        "ref_id": "FIGREF0"
                    }
                ],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "",
                "sec_num": null
            },
            {
                "text": "It proved to lack the robustness necessary to parse the MUC-3 messages, failing on 50% o f the sentences . For those sentences which it did parse, the Interpretext system returned precise semantic interpretations . ITP's word-based approach required minimal reorientation in shifting to the new domain of terrorism texts; the main new material was the straightforward addition of a relatively small number of new words to the syntactic and naive semantic lexicons, not whole ne w semantic modules . The semantic structures and analyses already implemented proved to be appropriate for texts in the new domain .",
                "cite_spans": [],
                "ref_spans": [],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "",
                "sec_num": null
            },
            {
                "text": "The source of the precision in our performance was the Cognitive Model built by the Natura l Language Understanding Module . The Cognitive Model contains specific reference marker s identifying events and individuals in the text . The same events and individuals are given the sam e reference markers by the Anaphora Resolution Module . The Cognitive Model distinguishe s between events, individuals and sets . It directly displays the argument structure of events . Thus , to find a terrorist incident, the template-filling code looked for an event which implied harm , damage or some other consequence of terrorism in the Naive Semantics for the verb naming th e event. The agent of the event had to be described as having a role in clandestine activity, th e government or the military . The ITP naive semantic lexicon distinguishes between nouns which names objects and nouns which name events, so that the template-filling code had only to look fo r events, even those introduced by phrases such as the destruction of homes in .. . Furthermore, the Cognitive Model connects head nouns with prepositional phrase modifier s and adjectival or nominal modifiers via the same reference marker . Thus the template-filling code could look for a variety of modifiers of an individual as a source of information about the individual . For example, the phrase member of the guerrilla troop connects member with troop and guerrilla, so that the template-filling code could recognize a semantically empty term lik e member as referring to an agent. This type of connection works everywhere, not just with the particular string pattern member of the guerrilla troop . Furthermore, it is much more precise than a pattern-matching method which would find guerrilla as perpetrator everywhere it occurs, eve n when a phrase like \"member of the guerrilla troop\" is the object of a verb which implies harm, and is therefore not indicative of guerrilla terrorism .",
                "cite_spans": [],
                "ref_spans": [],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "",
                "sec_num": null
            },
            {
                "text": "Another source of precision is that the formal semantic module interprets the cardinality o f sets . \"None\", \"plural\" or \"three\" come out in the formal representation as the number of objects i n a set. Finding target number and amount of injury and damage is trivial given a precise treatmen t of cardinality in the formal semantics.",
                "cite_spans": [],
                "ref_spans": [],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "",
                "sec_num": null
            },
            {
                "text": "Finally, the Cognitive Model indicated discourse segments . These are portions of the tex t which function as a unit around one topic . The recognition of segments simplified the anaphor a resolution and the process of identifying the same individuals and events with each other . It prevented the overgeneration of templates . Some competitor systems generated a new template for each sentence containing a terrorism word and then they had to try to merge them . Without segment information, merging was very difficult .",
                "cite_spans": [],
                "ref_spans": [],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "",
                "sec_num": null
            },
            {
                "text": "A Cognitive Model with this level of precision can be built only when a deep natural languag e analysis of the text is performed . Syntactic, formal semantic, discourse semantic and pragmatic (o r naive semantic) complexities of text are addressed by the ITP Natural Language Understandin g Module. Some researchers have rejected a principled linguistic approach as hopeless at this stag e in the history of computational linguistic research. They assume that the only feasible methods ar e statistical. Such systems match to certain string patterns and rely upon the statistical probability that they co-occur with a particular semantic interpretation . The problem is that many times th e pattern occurs in phrase which is irrelevant, or has the opposite meaning to the predicted one . The pattern can occur in the scope of a negative or modal, as in the bomb did not explode, and produce a false alarm for a pattern-matching method . Such methods will tend to over-generate templates , because patterns indicate a terrorist incident where there is none . For the same false alarm texts , more precise linguistic analysis can correctly rule out a terrorist incident .",
                "cite_spans": [],
                "ref_spans": [],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "",
                "sec_num": null
            },
            {
                "text": "Furthermore, the patterns for matching must be coded anew for each domain . In contrast , ITP Naive Semantic and syntactic lexicons need only be built once, and they work across al l domains. For MUC-3 we added to an existing naive semantic lexicon prepared originally for text s in other domains .",
                "cite_spans": [],
                "ref_spans": [],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "",
                "sec_num": null
            },
            {
                "text": "In summary, ITP was precise in the MUC-3 fills for the sentences which our loaner parser was able to process . When our own parser is available, ITP's technology will vastly improve i n recall.",
                "cite_spans": [],
                "ref_spans": [],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "",
                "sec_num": null
            },
            {
                "text": "The basic approach to template-filling involved looking at feature types in the naive semanti c knowledge for verbs and nouns . The feature types inspected had already been present in th e theory and in the system prior to MUC-3 . The verb feature \"consequence of event\" was importan t for recognizing terrorist incidents, because if the typical consequence of an event was damage or harm, it triggered a template fill . The theory of Naive Semantics as described in Dahlgren [1 ] identifies that feature type as important in lexical semantics and reasoning about discourse . Similarly, the \"rolein\" feature was used to distinguish between clandestine agents, governmen t agents and military agents . Again, that feature type was antecedently present in our theory .",
                "cite_spans": [
                    {
                        "start": 476,
                        "end": 480,
                        "text": "[1 ]",
                        "ref_id": "BIBREF0"
                    }
                ],
                "ref_spans": [],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "Naive Semantic s",
                "sec_num": null
            },
            {
                "text": "The effect of the MUC-3 reader was to exclude any sentences which did not contain a terro r word, saving processing time . This setting tended to reduce precision, because a sentence like She succeeded contains no terrorism word, but could be very significant in the recognition of a terroris t incident . Recall was implicitly set very low by the fact that the parser was able to parse only 50 % of the input.",
                "cite_spans": [],
                "ref_spans": [],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "Test Settings",
                "sec_num": null
            },
            {
                "text": "The greatest effort by ITP was the six years of research that went into the Natural Languag e Understanding Module . As for MUC-3-specific tasks, Table I indicates the level of effort on eac h one. ITP made a detailed linguistic analysis of the terrorism domain, and the way that terroris t incidents were described in the first messages sent out by NOSC, and in the DEV messages . The analysis guided the expansion of the lexicons and the writing of the template-filling code . During Test 1 we identified both parser failure and parse time to be problems in our performance . Therefore, for Test 2 we built a reader which could handle dates, abbreviations, and so on, an d would return a sentence only if it contained a terrorism word. In addition, we pruned the output to shorten sentences for the parser . These tactics will not be necessary once our own wide-coverag e parser is completed. The template-filling code took about as much of our time as the reader and pruner. Each element of the code reasons from the Cognitive Model using generalized lexica l reasoning or DRS reasoning . The temporal-locative reasoning is general and will be used in other applications. ",
                "cite_spans": [],
                "ref_spans": [
                    {
                        "start": 146,
                        "end": 153,
                        "text": "Table I",
                        "ref_id": null
                    }
                ],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "Level of Effort",
                "sec_num": null
            },
            {
                "text": "The main limiting factors were the parser and resources . With more persons and time, we could have written code for all of the fills and debugged the template-filling code thoroughly . Given the modest resources we had, we were forced to run the test before we had thoroughl y debugged the code . In particular, our code for recognizing and building up proper names was i n place, but failed during the test in most cases . That explained our performance on Perpetrato r Organization . Given that we missed the latter, we of course could not get Category of Inciden t correct for any of the State-sponsored Violence cases either .",
                "cite_spans": [],
                "ref_spans": [],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "Limiting Factor",
                "sec_num": null
            },
            {
                "text": "Training took place on the first 100 DEV messages, and on Test 1 messages with the ne w key. We did not have sufficient resources to fully debug and repeatedly test prior to MUC-3 week. The system improved dramatically between Test 1 and Test 2 (from recall of 3 to recall of 20) . Improvement was mainly due to expansion of the template-filling code and the introduction o f pruning to get more parses .",
                "cite_spans": [],
                "ref_spans": [],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "Training",
                "sec_num": null
            },
            {
                "text": "For those sentences which we were able to parse, the reasoning performed well for inciden t recognition, segmentation (separating different incidents in the same message), perpetrator an d target recognition . The only exceptions were perpetrators or targets with long proper names . We have an approach to these, but didn't get it working in time . The fills which failed were perpetrator organization (because of names), and target nationality . The latter code is working fine (it looks t o see whether any descriptor of an individual is a foreign nation name or adjective) . The failures were due to missing the whole template because of parsing, or missing the target in a recognize d template. In addition, our target number code was not fully operational at the time of the test . We would most like to rewrite the template-filling code in even more general reasoning algorithm s which could be used in applications beyond the terrorism domain . Our system's capabilities mak e possible a question-answering system which could reply to English queries like Who did it? and How many people were killed? .",
                "cite_spans": [],
                "ref_spans": [],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "Success and Failur e",
                "sec_num": null
            },
            {
                "text": "Everything but the template-filling code is reusable in a different application . All of th e words we added to the lexicons have all of their senses common in American English . They can be used in any domain. As for the template-filling code, we plan to extract generalizable reasonin g algorithms for use in other domains . Again, the code is reusable because it is a principled, general linguistic approach rather than a pattern-matching approach.",
                "cite_spans": [],
                "ref_spans": [],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "Reusability",
                "sec_num": null
            },
            {
                "text": "We learned that anything a person wants to say or write can be said in an extremely larg e number of different ways . Therefore, a robust deep natural language understanding system mus t have a wide-coverage parser and formal semantics which directly display the similarity of conten t across many possible forms of expression . A sound theoretical approach such as DRT i s particularly appropriate for a data extraction task. Secondly, we learned that natural language systems require ample testing against real-world texts . And, third, a system in which word meanings are central, developed to interpret text in the domains of geography and finance, can function in the domain of terrorism with the addition of a relatively small number of lexical items .",
                "cite_spans": [],
                "ref_spans": [],
                "eq_spans": [],
                "section": "What we learned",
                "sec_num": null
            }
        ],
        "back_matter": [],
        "bib_entries": {
            "BIBREF0": {
                "ref_id": "b0",
                "title": "Naive Semantics for Natural Language Understanding",
                "authors": [
                    {
                        "first": "K",
                        "middle": [],
                        "last": "Dahlgren",
                        "suffix": ""
                    }
                ],
                "year": 1988,
                "venue": "",
                "volume": "",
                "issue": "",
                "pages": "",
                "other_ids": {},
                "num": null,
                "urls": [],
                "raw_text": "Dahlgren, K. (1988) . Naive Semantics for Natural Language Understanding . Kluwer Academic Publishers, Norwell, Mass .",
                "links": null
            },
            "BIBREF1": {
                "ref_id": "b1",
                "title": "Coherence Relation Assignment",
                "authors": [
                    {
                        "first": "K",
                        "middle": [],
                        "last": "Dahlgren",
                        "suffix": ""
                    }
                ],
                "year": 1989,
                "venue": "Proceedings of the Cognitiv e Science Society",
                "volume": "",
                "issue": "",
                "pages": "588--596",
                "other_ids": {},
                "num": null,
                "urls": [],
                "raw_text": "Dahlgren,K. (1989) . \"Coherence Relation Assignment,\" in Proceedings of the Cognitiv e Science Society, pp.588-596 .",
                "links": null
            },
            "BIBREF2": {
                "ref_id": "b2",
                "title": "A Theory of Truth and Semantic Representation",
                "authors": [
                    {
                        "first": "H",
                        "middle": [],
                        "last": "Kamp",
                        "suffix": ""
                    }
                ],
                "year": 1981,
                "venue": "Formal Methods in the Study of Language, Mathematisch Centrum",
                "volume": "",
                "issue": "",
                "pages": "",
                "other_ids": {},
                "num": null,
                "urls": [],
                "raw_text": "Kamp, H. (1981) . \"A Theory of Truth and Semantic Representation, \" in Gronendijk, J . ; T. Janssen ; and M . Stokhof, editors, Formal Methods in the Study of Language, Mathematisch Centrum, Amsterdam .",
                "links": null
            }
        },
        "ref_entries": {
            "FIGREF0": {
                "type_str": "figure",
                "num": null,
                "uris": null,
                "text": "Intelligent Text Processing Final Scores Test 2 ITP's results are shown in"
            },
            "TABREF2": {
                "num": null,
                "content": "<table><tr><td>Tasks</td><td>Estimated Person-weeks</td></tr><tr><td>Linguistic analysis of terrorism domain</td><td>4</td></tr><tr><td>Syntactic Lexicon expansion</td><td>2</td></tr><tr><td>Naive Semantic Lexicon expansion</td><td>3</td></tr><tr><td>Reader, pruner</td><td>4</td></tr><tr><td>Temporal, locative reasoning</td><td>2</td></tr><tr><td>Template-filling code</td><td>4</td></tr></table>",
                "type_str": "table",
                "html": null,
                "text": "MUC-3 specific Tasks and their Estimated Person-Weeks"
            }
        }
    }
}